追逐即幸福——The Happiness of Pursuit

追逐即幸福——The Happiness of Pursuit

本文节选自《The Happiness of Pursuit》

作者Jeffrey Kluger,原载于美国《时代》杂志,2013年7月8日


Americans are free to pursue happiness, but there's no guarantee we'll achieve it. The secret is knowing how — and where —to look.

美国人生来就拥有追求幸福的自由,但是没人保证我们就一定能得到幸福。关键问题在于如何找到幸福。


*数字说

人际关系
“婚姻的确会提升幸福感,它是比钱或者小孩更好的幸福来源。”

“那些声称和父母关系良好的年轻人中,有80%的人日后的生活更幸福”

“那些专注于现在的人比沉浸在过去和未来中的人更幸福”

“在单身的人中有1/4的人认为婚姻会让他们的社交生活变得无聊”

“一项2013年的调查表明,出柜的男同性恋者比没出柜的更幸福”


工作和金钱

“一份烂工作也比没工作强。尽管他们的收入和工作时间都很差,但那些之前没工作的人找到工作后更幸福了。”

“那些挣15万的人声称自己过的开心的比率是那些挣2万或更少的人的两倍多。”

“人们买那些非必需品的时候更开心。和2012年相比,现在美国人把更多的钱花在买新车,出去吃饭和买比赛门票上了。”

“人们失业后的幸福感会大幅下跌,大部分是因为没有收入后导致的社会地位和自尊心的下降。”

“那些关心别人收入的人一般都过的比较不开心。”

 

*正文节选

All human beings may come equipped with the pursuit-of-happiness impulse--the urge to find lusher land just over the hill, fatter buffalo in the next valley--but it's Americans who have codified the idea, written it into the Declaration of Independence and made it a central mandate of the national character.

 

American happiness would never be about savor-the-moment contentment. That way lay the reflective café culture of the Old World--fine for Europe, not for James town. Our happiness would be bred, instead, of an almost adolescent restlessness, an itch to do the Next BigThing. The terms of the deal the founders offered are not easy: there's no guarantee that we'll actually achieve happiness, but we can go after it in almost any way we choose. All by itself, that freedom ought to bring us joy, but the more cramped, distracted, maddeningly kinetic nature of the modern world has made it harder than ever. Somehow there must be a way to thread that needle, to reconcile the contradictions between our pioneer impulses and our contemporary selves.

 

Those impulses are very deeply rooted: pilgrims to the New World were a self-selected group. Not every person suffering under the whip of tyranny or the crush of poverty had the temperamental wherewithal to pick up, pack up and travel to the other side of the globe and start over. Those who did were looking for something--pursuing something--and happiness is as good a way of defining that goal as any.

 

Consumptive happiness, the happiness that comes not from sowing but from reaping, not from building the house but from watching TV in your new living room. That may be the goal of the work, but it's a goal that, once achieved, can leave us feeling bored.

 

Since 1972, only about one-third of Americans have described themselves as "very happy," according to surveys funded by the National Science Foundation. Just since 2004, the share of Americans who identify themselves as optimists has plummeted from 79% to 50%, according to a new Time poll. Meanwhile, more than 20% of us will suffer from a mood disorder at some point in our lifetimes and more than 30% from an anxiety disorder. By the time we're 18 years old, 11% of us have been diagnosed with depression.

 

The pursuit of happiness, once an ideal, has become a big business but not an especially effective one; plenty of other countries are doing a lot better than we are without trying so hard. According to the 2012 World Happiness Report, published by the Earth Institute of Columbia University, the U.S. ranks 23rd on a 50-country happiness index,far behind No. 1 Iceland, No. 2 New Zealand and No. 3 Denmark and trailing Singapore, Malaysia, Tanzania and Vietnam.

 

If you're part of the demographic pursuing joy or just trying to quell some psychic angst, none of this is a surprise to you, nor is the way happiness is now being merchandised, since you may have spent more than your share of disposable income on meditation or yogaclasses, life coaching or happiness apps. Part of the solution, however, may lie not in a product or a program but simply in a better understanding of the particular way Americans define happiness in the first place. There are answers to be found in our genes, in our collective psyche, in the workings of our brain. If it was possible for our ancestors to be happy on the prairie, it ought to be possible for us to be happy in our jobs, our families, our communities. We've got all the toys; now we need to relocate the joy, to tap into the propensities that allow us to take pleasure in striving--in, if you will, the pursuit.

 

The Biology of Happy

天赋“快乐”?


That said, simple biology--evident since Gregor Mendel started breeding his pea plants in the 19th century--dictates that the random mix of genetic traits within any one population will be amplified when that population starts breeding. That ought to be true for so-called immigrant genes too, and in 2011, that idea got a big boost when investigators at Harvard and Boston University analyzed a gene dubbed DRD4, which is associated with activity in the brain's dopamine receptors. The gene comes in several forms, or alleles. Of the three most common, one codes for even-temperedness and reflection, while the other two code for exploratory and impulsive behavior, as well as a taste for risk taking and a tolerance of novelty.

 

When the investigators looked at the frequency of the different alleles in people around the world, they found that the farther along the migration route from Africa, the cradle of us all,through central Asia, Europe and the New World, the likelier people were to carry the two novelty-seeking alleles. Studies of another gene called 5-HTTLPR, related to serotonin transport, have yielded similar findings. The allele of that gene that codes for anxiety and risk avoidance is less common inindividualistic cultures like that of the U.S.

 

If genes play a role in shaping immigrant temperament, they do so in a subtle way. Serotonin and dopamine are often, simplistically, thought of as feel-good neurotransmitters. The more you have of them, the happier you are. But in the case of immigrants at least, the power of the chemicals is that they regulate what researchers straight forwardly call search activity--forward-looking behavior that often occurs in pursuit of a specific goal. Search activity simply feels good--a fact that helps explain why shopping for something is often more fun than buying it, hunting can be more enjoyable than actually bagging your prey, and so many politicians appear to have a better time running for office than holding it.

 

What's more, explains Dr. Vadim Rotenberg, a psychiatrist and psychophysiologist at Tel Aviv University, the feel-good search experience can stimulate people to continue pursuing a goal even when they're having trouble achieving it. That's as good an explanation for immigrant persistence as there ever was. So how does a brain bred for thejoy of pursuit react to stress and a climate of near constantdistractions--both grindingly consistent features of the postindustrial world?

 

At the neurological level, happinessis a very complex thing, and lots can go wrong. Studies of the brain conducted with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) show varying levels of happiness-related activity in the left prefrontal cortex and the more primitivebasal ganglia, which form part of the reward loop; the amygdala, which processes a range of basic emotions; the septal area, which is involved in the experience of empathy; and the anterior insula, which helps focus our attentionon the things that are making us happy in the first place.

 

Earlier this year, neuroscientist Sylvia Morelli of Stanford University and psychologist Matt Lieberman of UCLA used fMRIs to study how empathically people responded when they were looking at happy or sad images of other people. Empathic experiences are good proxies for personal ones because there's a lot of overlap in the regions of the brain in which they're processed; this is why sympathetic pain can make you squirm even though you haven't been injured and joy at a loved one's success can make youfeel as if you succeeded too.

 

In Morelli and Lieberman's study,the volunteers looked at the pictures either when they were free to focus on them completely or when they were trying to memorize an eight-digit number the researchers had assigned them. Consistently, the people operating under that so-called cognitive load showed reduced empathy reactions, with neural activity down across four different brain regions. People with uncluttered brains processed--and felt--things more deeply. "Being distracted reduces our empathy for others and blunts responses in the brain," says Morelli."So it's possible that being distracted may also reduce our own happiness." Memorizing an eight-digit number is hardly something you doevery day, but juggling e-mails, meeting deadlines and worrying about the next round of layoffs is, and that takes its toll.

 

 

 

Get Rich, Get Happy

金钱的确可以买来幸福,起码有时候是这样的

 

If tension is making us miserable,snuffing out all the good work our happy genes do, we've learned that one balm can fix it all: money. Never mind what you've been taught to the contrary, money can indeed buy happiness, at least in certain circumstances. It was in 1974 that University of Southern California economist Richard Easterlin first formulated his eponymous (and soon ubiquitous) Easterlin Paradox, which held that there is a threshold beyond which increases in income produce nocommensurate increase in subjective well-being. Once basic needs (food,clothing, shelter) are met, we simply reach a satiation point. For a lot of people, this never met the plausibility test, and for Americans in particular,who have always been unembarrassedly O.K. with the goal of getting rich, who delighted in a movie in which a character flatly announced that greed is good,the satiation idea was especially troubling.

 

Turns out we were right. The Easterlin Paradox held sway only until other researchers began poking at it,using longer-term data sets, testing them across multiple cultures and finding that while happiness may not rise as quickly as income (doubling your salary from $75,000 to $150,000 will not make you twice as happy) there is no such thing as growing numb to money. Indeed, just this April, a study by the BrookingsInstitution and the Gerald R. Ford School of Public Policy at the University of Michigan analyzed data from 155 countries and found that not only does subjective well-being rise along with income but in wealthy countries the slopeis actually sharper than it is in poorer countries. A 10% bump in a $ 50,000 income, for example, produces a greater happiness boost than a similar percentage increase in a $10,000 income, even though a little extra money at the lower end of the scale ought to have a more life-improving effect. Rich isn't just better; it's much better.

 

That, at least, is how things shak eout at the national and global level. At the individual and community level, it can be much different. If you're rich, your experiences are not the same as every other rich person's, and the same is true if you're poor. "A reporter once asked me, 'Yes or no, does money make people happy? No scientific waffling, just yes or no,'" says psychologist Edward Diener of the University of Illinois. "I hit Delete."

 

A massive study Diener led that was published last December analyzed the responses of 806,526 people in 135 countries collected over the course of six years. It found that income corresponds more or less directly to happiness but only if a person's wealth andaspirations keep pace. Earning $170,000 per year might put you in the top 5% of American households, but if you're dreaming of a one-percenter's lifestyle,you'll be disappointed. "Money can boost happiness if it allows people to obtain more of the things they need and desire," says Diener. "But when their desires outpace what they can afford, even rising income can be accompanied by falling feelings of well-being."

 

This is particularly problematic in the modern era. A century ago, every body knew the names of the country's richest families--the Carnegies, the Vanderbilts, the Astors--but they werelittle more than icons. You never really saw how they lived, which was just fine, since if you did, your little split-level house or sole-proprietor business would start to look pretty shabby. In an era of paparazzi and realityshows, everyone sees everything and almost all of us suffer by comparison withsomeone.

 

"Bertrand Russell used to say,'Beggars do not envy millionaires, though of course they will envy other beggars who are more successful,'" says psychologist Cameron Anderson of the University of California, Berkeley. Anderson studies the difference betweensocio economic status--a purely arithmetical measure of how much money you make--and sociometric status, which is a measure of how well you compare with the people around you. Before the beggar could see the millionaire, those distinctions were easier to draw. Now the silos have been blown up or at least made transparent, a process that has accelerated dramatically in the era of social media.

 

If you're on Facebook, there are more than 1.1 billion other people who can mainline their good times--their new car, their big house, their vacation that you'd have to save 10 years to take--straight into your brain. Half a billion people on Twitter can do the same, a punchy 140 characters at a time. The very set up of social media provides another way to keep score. You've got 50 Twitter followers? Great, but your best friend has 500, and Lady Gaga, in case you're counting, has 38 million. In the Time poll, 60% of respondents said they do not feel better about themselves after spending time on social media, and 76% believe other people make themselves look happier, more attractive and more successful thanthey actually are on their Facebook page.

 

"When it comes to hierarchies, people sort themselves into higher or lower positions," says Anderson."There's a line of research in which you make people feel high or low by imagining themselves with someone above them or below them." If most of the people in your virtual circle seem better off than you, there's no imagination necessary.

 

The irony is that those high-status folks may not feel much better than you, and not only because they too are always being exposed to someone who's better off than they are. Rather, their sense of well-being may hinge on why they're buying so many goodies and doing so much posting at all.

 

In 2012 psychologist Ryan Howell of San Francisco State University conducted a study of nearly 1,000 participants,administering a series of questionnaires about the things they buy, the reasonsthey buy them and what their level of happiness is. The more a purchase was motivated by an effort to impress other people, the study found, the less of a happiness boost it conferred. While most of us flatter ourselves that we'reabove that kind of crassness, consider that every vacation photo you everposted, every new article of clothing you imagined wearing into the office evenas you were paying for it, every new car you bought and parked conspicuously inyour driveway instead of invisibly in your garage was motivated by the same look-at-me impulse. In a wealthy culture like ours, there's a lot of opportunity for that kind of exhibitionistic spending, as well as for the let down that follows when the happiness never comes.

 

In those cases, Howell says,"it's as if your values and what you're interested in don't matter. You can think of it as a litmus test: Would you still engage in this experience if you could tell no one about it?"

 

Howell is expanding his database with the help of an interactive website, BeyondthePurchase.org which allows users to takesurveys about their buying practices. Their responses are lending support tothe idea that another mistake we make is choosing to buy things instead ofexperiences. Your shoes are not unique; your TV's not unique. Your vacation to Rome or your family camping trip, however, are much more particularly yourssince nobody else in the world did exactly the same things or shared them withexactly the same people you did. And far from wearing out, the memories of theexperience grow richer over time. "Money can make you happy," Howellsays. "But it's about how you spend it."

当前位置

测评中心

Oscar 数字测评中心

奥斯卡数字测评中心™ We believe 测评的目的不是提供见解,而是答案! 更多>